MASSED
Vs SPACED LEARNING
INTRODUCTION:
Most of us would like to have a better memory. The
ability to remember things accurately and effortlessly would make us more
efficient in our daily lives, and it would make us more successful in our work.
The most obvious requirement for learning something new is practice. One basic
question which applies to most learning situations is whether it is better to
do the learning in one large "cramming" session. These two approaches
are known as "massed" and "spaced" learning, respectively,
and they are illustrated in Figure 1.1,
Figure
1.1 Massed and Spaced learning sessions
It
has generally been found that spaced learning is
more efficient than massed learning. This was first demonstrated
more than a century ago by Ebbinghaus (1885), who found that spaced
learning sessions produced higher retrieval scores than massed
learning sessions, when the total time spent learning was
kept constant for both learning conditions. Ebbinghaus used lists of nonsense syllables as his test
material, but the general superiority of spaced over
massed learning has been confirmed by many subsequent
studies using various different types of test material, such as learning
lists of words (Dempster, 1987), sentences (Rothkopf & Coke,
1963) and text passages (Reder & Anderson, 1982). Spaced
learning has also generally proved to be better than massed
learning when learning motor skills, such as learning pursuit
rotor skills (Bourne & Archer, 1956) and learning keyboard
skills (Baddeley & Longman, 1978).
Most early studies of spaced learning involved
the use of uniformly spaced learning sessions. However, Landauer and Bjork
(1978) found that learning is often more efficient if the time interval between retrieval
sessions is steadily increased for successive sessions. This strategy is known
as "expanding retrieval practice".
Although
spaced learning has been consistently found to be
superior to massed learning over the long term, it has been found
that during a learning session (and for a short time afterwards) massed
learning can actually produce better retrieval than spaced
learning, and the advantage of spaced learning only
really becomes apparent with longer retrieval intervals (Glenberg & Lehman,
1980).
Although it has been demonstrated that spaced
learning sessions are usually more effective than massed
learning sessions, in real-life settings this advantage may sometimes be
compromised by practical considerations. A further problem is that spaced
learning obviously requires more time overall (i.e. total time including
rest breaks) than massed learning, and therefore may not
represent the most efficient use of that time unless the rest breaks can be
used for something worthwhile. Because spaced learning can
create practical problems of this kind, there is no clear agreement about its
value in a real-life learning setting such as a school classroom. Dempster (1988) suggests that
teachers should make use of the spaced learning principle,
whereas spaced learning is not really practicable in a classroom
setting, since the periodic interruption of learning session can be
inconvenient and can make learning less pleasant. On balance it can be argued
that spaced learning is probably the best option for most simple learning tasks
so long as we fit the sessions around our other activities, but it may not be
practicable in some settings such as the school classroom.
References:
Esgate, A. (2004). An Introduction to Applied Cognitive
Psychology, Psychology Press: Taylor & Francis Group, UK. 10-12.
Ebbinghaus, H. (1885). Memory: A contribution to experimental
psychology. New York: Dover.
Dempster, F. N. (1987). Effects of variable encoding and spaced
presentation on vocabulary learning . Journal of Educational Psychology 79:
162-170.
Rothkopf, E. Z., & Coke, E. U. (1963). Repetition interval and
rehearsal method in learning equivalences from written sentences. Journal of
Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 2, 406-416.
Reder, L. M., & Anderson, J. R. (1982). Effects of spacing and
embellishment on memory for the main points of a text. Memory & Cognition,
10, 97-102.
Landauer, T. K., & Bjork, R. A. (1978). Optimum rehearsal
patterns and name learning. In M. M. Gruneberg, P. E. Morris, & R. N.
Sykes, (Eds.), Practical Aspects of Memory (pp. 625-632). London: Academic
Press.
Glenberg, A. M., & Lehmann, T. S. (1980). Spacing repetitions
over 1 week. Memory & Cognition, 8, 528-538.
Dempster, F. N. (1988). The spacing effect: A case study in the
failure to apply the results of psychological research. American Psychologist,
43, 627-634.
Bourne, L.E., Jr., & Archer, E.J. (1956). Time continuously on
target as a function of distribution of practice. Journal of Experimental
Psychology, 51, 25-33.
Baddeley, A. D., & Longman, D. J. A. (1978). The influence of
length and frequency of training session on the rate of learning to type.
Ergonomics, 21, 627-635.
No comments:
Post a Comment