Cognitive
Style:
The area was developed by Herman Witkin. His
research showed that there were differences in how people perceived discrete
items within a surrounding field.
Field
dependence-independence describes the extent to which:
· The surrounding framework dominates the
perception of items within it,
· The surrounding organized field influences a
person's perception of items within it,
· A person perceives part of the field as a
discrete form,
· The organization of the prevailing field
determines the perception of its components, or · A person perceives
analytically. (Jonassen & Grabowski,
p. 86)
When field dependents interact with stimuli,
they find it difficult to locate the information they are seeking because other
information masks what they are looking for.
Field independents find it easier to recognize and select the important
information from its surrounding field. When information is presented in an
ambiguous, unstructured format, the field independent willimpose his/her own
structure on the information. The field
dependent will attempt to understand and learn that information as it is
presented and without restructuring it.
Another way to look at field dependence and
independence is through a global versus articulated cognitive style. Those with a global perspective, field
dependents, see things in the entire perceptual field (the forest rather than
the trees). In other words, field dependents have difficulty separating the
part from the complex organization of the whole. The analytic style presented
by field independents allows them to create their own models for things they
want to understand or articulate to others.
Witkin combined the various dimensions of social
and intellectual behavior into a Theory of Psychological Differentiation
(Witkin, et.al., 1962) which includes four dimensions: global-articulate,
articulation of body concept, sense of identity, and defense structures. The most important aspect of Witkin's Theory
is his belief that these are stable traits that predict cognitive and social
functioning across environments.
Two
types of cognitive styles:
1)
Field independent (analytic): objects in
one’s environment are experienced as separate & distinct from their
surroundings; thus they can easily be disembedded from their context
2)
Field dependent (holistic): one’s
environment is experienced as a unity of objects; an emphasis on the holistic
intermingling of parts
Field
Independence—Field Dependence
The
work of Witkin. Goodenough, and Cox (1977) on field independence-field
dependence closely parallels brain hemispherical findings. These
researchers described learners as field independent
(having the ability to perceive objects without being influenced by the
background) and field dependent (having the ability to perceive objects
as a whole rather than as individual parts). Characteristics of field-independent
and field-dependent learners are summarized in the following
chart.
Type
of Learner
Characteristics
|
|
Field
independent
|
Processes
Information in pans
Might
locus on specific parts, rather than see the whole
Passive
In social situations
Tends
to be less influenced by peers
Likes
working alone
Chooses
fields like main, science, and engineering
|
Field dependent
|
Processes
information holistically
Has
difficulty separating specific pans from a situation or pattern Able to see
relational concepts Active in social situations
Tends
to be influenced by suggestions from others Likes 10 work In groups
Chooses
fields requiring Interpersonal, nonscientific orientation, such as history,
an. or social work
|
Field
independence vs. Field Dependence
There
has been a growing body of research in the past decades since the field
dependence-independence dimension was first proposed by Witkin (1962; 1979).
According to Witkin, field dependence-independence is value-neutral and is
characterized as the ability to distinguish key elements from a distracting or
confusing background. Field dependence-independence has important implications
for an individual’s cognitive behavior and for his/her interpersonal behavior.
Specifically, field independent people tend to be more autonomous in relation
to the development of cognitive restructuring skills and less autonomous in
relation to the development of interpersonal skills. Conversely, field
dependent people tend to be more autonomous in relation to the development of
high interpersonal skills and less autonomous in relation to the development of
cognitive restructuring skills. In addition, according to Witkin, Moore,
Goodenough, and Cox (1977), field independent persons tend to be intrinsically
motivated and enjoy individualized learning, while field dependent ones tend to
be extrinsically motivated and enjoy cooperative learning.
The
field independence dimension is also related to some other individual
characteristics, such as solving analogical problems. According to Antonietti
and Gioletta (1995), cognitive styles, rather than general abilities, are
related to analogical problem solving. Antonietti and Gioletta found that field
independent participants were more likely to be analogical solvers than field
dependent ones. Males tended to use analogical solutions more frequently than
females. In addition, according to Braune and Wickens (1986), there are three
important dimensions of individual differences in time-sharing–serial
processing, parallel processing, and the internal model. Field independent
persons perform better in the parallel processing conditions, while field
dependent ones perform better in the serial processing conditions.
The
field independence dimension is also related to some task characteristics.
According to Bennink (1982), high and low field articulation (FA) students show
differences in the following two major respects under cognitively demanding
conditions: (a) integrating a set of semantically related sentences to answer
inference questions and (b) remembering the actual propositions themselves.
Assessment
of field-dependency was further developed to include several variations of the
original tests. A pencil and paper assessment, the Embedded Figures Test (EFT),
was developed reflecting earlier work on the discrimination of shape carried
out by Thurstone (1944). All of these measures involve the disembodying of a
shape from its surrounding field. This assessment included the following tests.
1. The
Embedded Figures Test (EFT): a 12-item test, made up of two sets of cards
displaying complex figures and simple figures respectively.
2. Children's
Embedded Figures Test (CEFT): a 25-item, individually administered test
that combines a series of simple and complex figures, and incomplete pictures
requiring the subject to disembed or recognize embedded shapes. The test was
norm-referenced with children from 5 to 12 years of age (Witkin et al., 1971).
3. Group Embedded Figures Test (GEFT): a
group-administered, 25-item test for adults in which the format is very similar
to the EFT (Witkin et al.).
repression-sensitization
a
dimension of differences in defensive patterns of perception, ranging from
avoiding the anxiety-arousing stimuli to approaching them more readily and
being extra-vigilant or supersensitized
---in
spite of disagreement about the concepts and the role of repression, there is
now agreement that people use a mechanism of mentally distracting themselves
from awareness of threatening and negative thoughts or emotions
people
who describe themselves as having few problems or difficulties and who do not
report themselves as highly sensitive to everyday stress and anxieties; listed
themselves as having few problems
repressors
---sensitive
to criticism, did not attend to negativity
--emotional
blunting for everyday information (not massive repression) may be an actual
sign of mental health (distracting self)
sensitizers
individuals
who are highly sensitive to everyday stress and anxiety; report many problems
about their lives of self-reports;
--attended
to negative information; more negative self-description
--technically
more mentally healthy as they are not repressing.
The
term perceptual defense has subsequently evolved into the term
"repression, " and the term perceptual vigilance has evolved into the
term "sensitization." Since the sensitizer, perceptual vigilant,
recognizes the threatening stimuli earlier than the neutral stimuli, this
perceptual style was also characterized as one of "approach," while
the style of the repressor, perceptual defender, which revealed the opposite
relationship was characterized as one of "avoidance."
Both
of these perceptual modes share the fact that they involve differential
awareness to threatening stimuli. Consequently, both have been thought to
represent polar opposites in the handling of the anxiety or fear that is
aroused by aversive stimuli. Hence, the perceptual styles have also been
thought of as defense mechanisms, while their polarity has been considered to
anchor an approach-avoidance dimension of individual reactivity to aversive
stimuli.
Levelling-Sharpening
A tendency to
assimilate detail rapidly and lost or emphasize details and changes in new
information.
Leveller
and sharpener
Holzman
and Klein (1954) first used the terms "levelling" and
"sharpening" to describe individual differences in memory processing.
Gardner, Jackson, and Messick (1960) described this cognitive process as
"the characteristic degree to which current precepts and relevant memory
traces interact or assimilate in the course of registration of the current
precepts and memories" (p. 122). The distinction between levellers
and sharpeners is based on how the task is perceived.
Holzman and Klein noted that while some individuals oversimplify their perceptions
(levelling), others have a tendency to perceive the task in a complex and
differentiated fashion, showing little assimilation (sharpening).
While
levellers tend to assimilate new events with previously stored ones, sharpeners,
in contrast, tend to accentuate the perceived events and treat
them more discretely from those already stored. How well the perceived task is
assimilated led to the notion of a dimension of assimilation, with sharpeners
at one end, showing very little assimilation, to levellers
at the other end, showing high levels of assimilation. The
continuum from levelling to sharpening is described as a dimension of
cognitive style, which alters during maturation, to reflect a movement away
from levelling toward sharpening. This involves, more specifically, a movement
away from fluid to a stable memory structure, as well as from a global to an
articulated differentiation of past and present images and events.
It
has been noted that this model of cognitive style has
two main forms of manifestation:
• a
tendency to "gloss over" inconsistencies, or;
• the condensing of information, involving simplification on the
one hand, and/or generalization involving caricature on the other.
Pain is an
unpleasant sensory and emotional experience associated with
actual or potential tissue damage (Merskey, 1986).
Theories of Pain
Perception
Specificity
Theory
Specificity
theory is one of the first modern theories for pain. It holds that specific
pain receptors transmit signals to a "pain center" in the brain that
produces the perception of pain. This theory is correct in that separate fibers
for pain signals do carry pain signals to the brain eventually. However, the
theory does not account for the wide range of psychological factors that affect
our perception of pain. For example, soldiers may report little or no pain in
relation to a serious wound in war time that would otherwise be excruciating.
Pattern Theory
Pattern
theory holds that pain signals are sent to the brain only when stimuli sum
together to produce a specific combination or pattern. The theory does not
posit specialized receptors for pain nor does it see the brain as having
control over the perception of pain. Rather, the brain is merely viewed as a
message recipient. Despite its limitations, the Pattern Theory did set the
stage for the Gate Control theory that has proved the most influential and best
accepted pain theory so far.
Gate Control
Theory
Ronald
Melzack and Patrick Wall proposed the Gate Control Theory in 1965. The theory
can account for both "top-down" brain influences on pain perception
as well as the effects of other tactile stimuli (e.g. rubbing a banged knee) in
appearing to reduce pain. It proposed that there is a "gate" or
control system in the dorsal horn of the spinal cord through which all
information regarding pain must pass before reaching the brain. The Substantia
Gelatinosa (SG) in the dorsal horn controls whether the gate is open or closed.
An "open gate" means that the transmission cells (i.e., t-cells) can
carry signals to the brain where pain is perceived; a "closed gate"
stops the t-cells from firing and no pain signal is sent to brain.
No comments:
Post a Comment