Information, Memory, and Thinking
The information
•processing approach emphasizes that children manipulate information, monitor
it, and strategize about it. Central to this approach arc the processes of
memory and thinking. According to the information-processing approach, children
develop a gradually increasing capacity for processing information, which
allows them to acquire increasingly complex knowledge and skills (Halford,
2008).
Behaviorism and
its associative model of learning was a dominant force in psychology until the
1950s and 1960s, when many psychologists began to acknowledge that they could
not explain children's learning without referring to mental processes such as
memory and thinking The term cognitive psychology became a label for
approaches that sought to explain behavior by examining mental
processes. Although a number of factors stimulated the growth of cognitive
psychology, none was more important than the development of computers. The
first modern computer, developed by John von Neumann in the late 1940s,
showed that inanimate machines could perform logical operations. This suggested
that some mental operations might be carried out by computers, possibly
telling us something about the way human cognition works. Cognitive
psychologists often draw analogies to computers to help explain the relation
between cognition and the brain (Robinson-Riegler 8r Robinson-Riegler, 2008).
The physical brain is compared with the computers hardware, cognition with its
software. Although computers and software aren't perfect analogies for brains
and cognitive activities, nonetheless, the comparison contributed to our
thinking about the child's mind as an active Information-processing system.
Cognitive Resources: Capacity and Speed of
Processing Information
As children grow
and mature, and as they experience the world, their information-processing
abilities increase. These changes are likely influenced by increases in both
capacity and speed of processing (Frye, 2004). These two characteristics are
often referred to as cognitive resources, which are proposed to have an
important influence on memory and problem solving.
Both biology and
experience contribute to growth in cognitive resources. Think about how much
faster you can process information in your native language than in a second
language. The changes in the brain we described in Chapter 2 provide a
biological foundation for increased cognitive resources. As children grow and
mature, important biological developments occur both in brain structures, such
as changes in the frontal lobes, and at the level of neurons, such as the
blooming and pruning of connections between neurons that produces fewer but
stronger connections (Kuhn. 2008; Nelson. 2009). Also, as we discussed in
Chapter 2. myelinatiom (the process that covers the axon with a myelin sheath)
increases the speed of electrical impulses in the brain Myelination continues through childhood and
adolescence (Spear. 2007).
Mod
information-processing psychologists argue that an increase in capacity also
improves processing of information (Mayer, 2008). For example, as children's in
formation-processing capacity increases, they likely can hold in mind several
dimensions of a topic or problem simultaneously, whereas younger children are
more prone to focus on only one dimension. Adolescents can discuss how the
varied experiences of the Founding Fathers influenced the Declaration of
Independence and Constitution. Elementary-age children are more likely to focus
on simple facts about the founders' lives.
What is the role
of processing speed? How fast children process information often influences
what they can do with that information. If an adolescent is trying to add up
mentally the cost of items he is buying at the grocery store, he needs
to be able to compute the sum before he has forgotten the price of the
individual items. Children's speed in processing information is linked with
their competence in thinking (Bjorklund. 2005). For example, how fast children
an articulate a series of words affects how many words they can store and
remember. Generally, fast processing is linked with good performance on
cognitive tasks. However, some compensation for slower processing speed can be
achieved through effective strategies.
Researchers have
devised a number of ways for assessing processing speed. For example, it can be
assessed through a reaction-lime task in which individuals are asked to
push a button as soon as they sec a stimulus such as a light. Or individuals
might be asked to match letters or numbers with symbols on a computer screen.
There is abundant
evidence that the speed with which such tasks arc completed improves
dramatically across the childhood years (Kail, 2007; Luna & others, 2004;
Mabbolt & others. 2006). Processing speed continues lo improve in early
adolescence. For example, in one study, 10-ycar-olds were approximately 1.8
times slower at processing information than young adults on such (asks as
reaction time, letter matching, mental rotation, and abstract matching (Hale.
1990). Twelve-year-olds were approximately 1.5 times slower than young adults,
but 15-year-olds processed information on the tasks as fast as the young
adults. Also, a recent study 8- to 13
year-old children revealed that processing speed increased with age, and
further that the developmental change in processing speed increased in working
memory. There is controversy about whether the increase In processing speed is
due to experience or biological maturation. Experience dearly plays an
important role. Think how much faster you could process the answer to a simple
arithmetic problem as an adolescent than as a child. Also think about how much
faster you can process Information in your native language than in a second
language. The role of biological maturation likely involves myelinalion.
Mechanisms of
Change
According to Robert Stegler 11998), three mechanisms work together to
create changes in children's cognitive skills: encoding, automaticity, and
strategy construction.
Encoding is the process by
which information gets stored in
memory. Changes in children's cognitive skills depend on increased skill at
encoding relevant information and ignoring irrelevant information. For
example, lo a 4-year-old, an s
in cursive writing is a shape
very different from an s (hat is printed. But a 10-year-old has learned to encode
the relevant fact that both arc the letter s and to ignore the irrelevant
differences in their shape.
Automaticity refers to the ability lo process information with little or no effort.
Practice allows children to encode increasing amounts of information
automatically. For example, once children have learned lo read well, they do
not think about each letter in a word as a letter; instead, they encode whole words. Once a task is automatic, ii docs not require
conscious effort. As a result, as information processing becomes more
automatic, we can complete tasks more quickly and handle more than one task at
a lime (Mayer, 2008; Schraw. 2006). Imagine how long it would take you to read
this page if you did not encode words automatically but instead focused your
attention on each letter in each word.
Strategy
construction is the creation of new procedures for processing
information. For example, children's reading benefits when they develop the
strategy of slopping periodically to take stock of what they have read so far.
Developing an effective repertoire of strategies and selecting the best one to
use on a learning task is a critical aspect of becoming an effective learner
(Pressley, 2007; Pressley 8t Harris, 2006).
In addition to these mechanisms of change, children's information
processing is characterized by self -modification
That is, children learn to use what they have
learned in previous circumstances to adapt their responses lo a new situation.
For example, a child who is familiar with dogs and cats goes to the zoo and
sees lions and tigers for the first time. She then modifies her concept of
"animal" to include her new knowledge. Part of this self-modification
draws on metacognition, which means "knowing about knowing". One
example of metacognition is what children know about the best ways to remember
what they have read. Do they know that they will remember what (hey have read
better if they can relate it to their own lives in some way? Thus, in Siegler's
application of information processing to development, children play an active
role in their cognitive development when they develop metacognitive strategies.
No comments:
Post a Comment