The Prototype Approach
According
to a theory proposed by Eleanor Rosch, we organize each category on the basis
of a prototype, which
is the item that is most typical and representative of the category. According
to this prototype approach, you
decide whether an item belongs to a category by comparing that item with a
prototype. If the item is similar to the prototype, you include that item in
the category.
In
some cases, the prototype of a category may not even exist. Rosch (1973) also
emphasizes that members of a category differ in their prototypicality, or degree
to which they are prototypical. A robin and a sparrow are very prototypical
birds, whereas ostriches and penguins are nonprototypes.
The
prototype approach represents a different perspective from the feature
comparison Model. According to the feature comparison model, an item belongs to
a category as long as it possesses the necessary and sufficient features
(Markman, 1999). The feature comparison perspective therefore argues that category
membership is very clear-cut. For example, for the category “bachelor,” the
defining features are male and unmarried.
Eleanor
Rosch and her coauthors, as well as other researchers, have conducted numerous
studies on the characteristics of prototypes. Their research demonstrates that
all members of categories are not created
equal. Instead, a category tends to have a graded
structure, beginning with the most representative
or prototypical members and continuing on through the category’s nonprototypical
members.
Characteristics of Prototypes. Prototypes
differ from the nonprototypical members of categories in several respects.
1.
Prototypes are supplied as examples of
a category. Several studies have shown that people
judge some items to be better examples of a concept than other items.
2.
Prototypes are judged more quickly
after semantic priming. The semantic priming effect means
that people respond faster to an item if it was preceded by an item with
similar meaning.
3.
Prototypes share attributes in a family
resemblance category. Before we examine this
issue, let’s introduce a new term, family resemblance. Family resemblance means that
no single attribute is shared by all examples of a concept; however, each
example has at least one attribute in common with some other example of the
concept.
Cognitive neuroscience is an
academic field concerned with the scientific study of biological substrates underlyingcognition,[1] with a specific focus on the neural substrates of mental processes. It
addresses the questions of how psychological/cognitive functions are produced
by neural circuits in the brain. Cognitive neuroscience is a branch of both psychology and neuroscience,
overlapping with disciplines such as physiological psychology, cognitive
psychologyand neuropsychology.[2] Cognitive neuroscience relies upon
theories in cognitive science coupled with evidence from
neuropsychology and computational
modeling.
Methods
employed in cognitive neuroscience include experimental paradigms from psychophysics and cognitive psychology, functional
neuroimaging, electrophysiology, cognitive
genomics and behavioral
genetics.
Cognitive
neuroscience can look at the effects of damage to the brain and subsequent
changes in the thought processes due to changes in neural circuitry resulting
from the ensued damage. Also, cognitive abilities based on brain development is
studied and examined under the subfield of developmental cognitive neuroscience.
“Cognitive Psychology is the study of thinking and the
processes underlying mental events.”
No comments:
Post a Comment