Models of Attention
Early selection theories
Broadbent’s model of selective attention or Filter theory
The theories described here belong to the
group of filter and bottleneck theories. A filter blocks some of the
information going through and thereby selects only a part of the total of
information to pass through to the next stage. A bottleneck slows down
information passing through. The models differ in two ways. First, do they have
a distinct “filter” for incoming information? Second, if they do, where in the
processing of information does the filter occur (early or late)?
Broadbent’s
Model
According to one of the earliest theories of attention, we filter
information right after we notice it at the sensory level. Multiple channels of
sensory input reach an attentional filter. Those channels can be distinguished
by their characteristics like loudness, pitch, or accent.
The filter permits only one channel of sensory information to
proceed and reach the processes of perception. We thereby assign meaning to our
sensations. Other stimuli will be filtered out at the sensory level and may
never reach the level of perception.
Broadbent’s
theory was supported by Colin Cherry’s findings
that sensory information sometimes may be noticed by an unattended ear if it
does not have to be processed elaborately (e.g., you may notice that the voice
in your unattended ear switches to a tone). But information requiring higher
perceptual processes is not noticed if not attended to (e.g., you would likely
not notice that the language in your unattended ear switches from English to
German).
Selective
Filter Model
The evidence began to suggest that Broadbent’s model must be
wrong. Moray found that even when
participants ignore most other high-level (e.g., semantic) aspects of an
unattended message, they frequently still recognize their names in an
unattended ear. He suggested that the reason for this effect is that messages
that are of high importance to a person may break through the filter of
selective attention. But other messages may not. To modify Broadbent’s
metaphor, according to Moray, the
selective filter blocks out most information at the sensory level. But some
personally important messages are so powerful that they burst through the
filtering mechanism.
Attenuation
Model
To explore why some unattended messages pass through the filter, Anne Treisman conducted some
experiments. She had participants shadowing coherent messages, so they must
have been somehow processing the content of the unattended message.
Moray’s modification of Broadbent’s filtering mechanism was
clearly not sufficient to explain Treisman’s findings. Her findings suggested
that at least some information about unattended signals is being analyzed.
Treisman proposed a theory of selective attention that involves a later
filtering mechanism (Figure 4.9). Instead of blocking stimuli out, the filter
merely weakens (attenuates) the strength of stimuli other than the target
stimulus. So when the stimuli reach us, we analyze them at a low level for target
properties, we pass the signal on to the next stage; if they do not possess those
target properties, we pass on a weakened version of the stimulus. In a next
step, we perceptually analyze the meaning of the stimuli and their relevance to
us, so that even a message from the unattended ear that is supposedly
irrelevant can come into consciousness and influence our subsequent actions if
it has some meaning for us.
Late-Filter
Model
Deutsch
and Deutsch developed a model in which the location of the filter is even
later. They suggested that stimuli are filtered out only after they have been
analyzed for both their physical properties and their meaning. This later
filtering would allow people to recognize information entering the unattended
ear.
Note that proponents of both the early and the late-filtering
mechanisms propose that there is an attentional bottleneck through which only a
single source of information can pass. The two models differ only in terms of
where they hypothesize the bottleneck to be positioned.
Late selection theories
Neisser’s synthesis model
A
Synthesis of Early-Filter and Late-Filter Models
Both early and late selection theories have data to support attention
models. In 1967, Ulric Neisser
synthesized the early-filter and the late-filter models and proposed that
there are two processes governing attention:
• Preattentive
processes: These automatic processes are rapid and occur in parallel.
They can be used to notice only physical
sensory characteristics of the unattended message. But they do not discern
meaning or relationships.
• Attentive,
controlled processes: These processes occur later. They are executed
serially and consume time and attentional resources, such as working memory.
They also can be used to observe relationships
among features. They serve to synthesize fragments into a mental representation
of an object.
A two-step model could account for Cherry’s, Moray’s, and
Treisman’s data. The model also nicely incorporates aspects of Treisman’s
signal-attenuation theory and of her subsequent feature-integration theory.
Feature‐Integration
Theory
Treisman (1980) started this theory with simple foundations,
however the current form of the theory is complex and complicated. Three
different aspects
of the theory are considered.
a.
Basic elements of the theory
b.
Research on the theory
c.
Current status of the theory
Basic Elements
1. Treisman suggests that we look at a scene, two
kinds of attentional processes take place.
2. In the beginning, all parts of the scene are
processed at the same time called distributed attention (divided attention). Such attention is
automatic, effortless, fast and is carried out as a
parallel process.
3. The second kind is focused attention, which involves consciously driven, effortful, slow processing
carried out as a serial process.
Research on Theory
1. Treisman suggests that we look at a scene, two
kinds of attentional processes take place.
2. In the beginning, all parts of the scene are
processed at the same time called distributed attention (divided attention). Such attention is
automatic, effortless, fast and is carried out as a
parallel process.
3. The second kind is focused attention, which involves consciously driven, effortful, slow processing
carried out as a serial process.
Illusory
Conjunctions
1. Later research showed that we make in search when two features
in two objects are confused. Presented with blue N and green T, results in blue
T.
2. For our auditory sense we make similar errors. Given “dax” and “kay” as nonsense syllables
participants respond by the word “day”.
3. Such errors (illusory conjunctions) are
cussed by object proximity or if there is distraction in attention to begin
with.
4. Presence of illusory conjunctions suggests that
features are processed separately and then put together later in our minds (binding problem).
5. Lack of focused attention leads to illusory
conjunctions and thus form the basis of integrated perception of features or
binding problem.
Current
Status of the Theory
1. Over 25 years Feature‐integration theory has gone through major
revisions and has become a complex theory.
2. Errors based on illusory conjunctions can be reduced with
practice (Coren et al., 2004; Treisman, 1992)
3. For some tasks distributed and focused attentional processes
loose distinction.
4. Theory awaits new neurophysiological insights. However serves
as a strong framework to explain visual attention.
No comments:
Post a Comment